Flower_constancy

Flower constancy

Flower constancy

Tendency to visit certain flower species


Flower constancy or pollinator constancy is the tendency of individual pollinators to exclusively visit certain flower species or morphs within a species, bypassing other available flower species that could potentially contain more nectar.[1][2][3] This type of foraging behavior puts selective pressures on floral traits in a process called pollinator-mediated selection. Flower constancy is different from other types of insect specialization such as innate preferences for certain colors or flower types, or the tendency of pollinators to visit the most rewarding and abundant flowers.[3]

Flower constancy has been observed for insect pollinators: especially honeybees (Apis mellifera),[4] a bumblebee (Bombus terrestris),[5][6] and a butterfly (Thymelicus sylvestris).[7] For example, honeybees have demonstrated a preference for certain flower types and constantly return even if other more rewarding flowers are available. This is shown for example in experiments where honeybees remain flower constant and do not attempt to feed on other available flowers that exhibit an alternative color to their preferred flower type.[4]

Adaptive behavior

Flower constancy favors flower pollination, that is, pollinators that are flower constant are more likely to transfer pollen to other conspecific plants.[8] Also, flower constancy prevents the loss of pollen during interspecific flights and pollinators from clogging stigmas with pollen of other flower species.[2] Flower constancy can be enhanced when the flowers are more dissimilar, for example in their coloration.[9] When, in a community of flowering plants, the flowers are all similarly colored, the constancy is often lower because different species are more difficult to distinguish, whereas constancy tends to be higher when the flowers are distinctly differently colored.[9]

Flower constancy benefits flower pollination but, arguably, constancy is not so obviously adaptive for pollinators. Individuals that show constant behavior ignore other flowers that could potentially provide more nectar (reward) than their preferred type. As a result, flower constancy seems to contradict optimal foraging models, which assume that animals will move minimal distances between food resources and so will feed on a mixture of these to maximize their energy intake per unit time.[2] As a result of this apparent contradiction, many hypotheses have been proposed to explain flower constancy in insects to determine the adaptability of flower constancy. One of the most popular explanations is that insects that are flower constant have limited memory space and can only focus on one flower type at a time.

Hypotheses

Memory limitation

Insects, as with other animals, have short-term memory or "working memory" where information is stored temporarily for a few second or minutes. Additionally, insects have long-term memory or "reference memory", which stores information for hours or more. One of the most widespread explanations for flower constancy is that insects can only identify and handle one flower type or species at a time.[3] Conversely, other scientists argue that insects such as bees can store large amounts of information (location of nest, flower patches, and existence of surrounding landmarks) in their long-term memory.[2]

Learning investment

The learning investment hypothesis relates to the ability of an insect to learn a motor skill to handle and obtain nectar from a flower type or species. Learning these motor skills could require substantial investment; switching to other flower types or species could be inefficient and consequently non-adaptive. Concentrating and feeding on one particular flower type would then increase the insect's efficiency of obtaining nectar.[2]

Costly information

The costly information hypothesis explains flower constancy by supposing that insects stay constant to a one flower type because they know they are obtaining a reliable reward: nectar. The insect does not venture to feed on other flower types because it cannot predict the amount of nectar in other flowers, and could waste foraging time probing less productive types of flower.[2]

Resource-partitioning

In social foragers, flower constancy could benefit the colony in that foragers avoid competition by specializing on a specific flower type. In this case, an individual insect, for example a bee, becomes flower constant to avoid competition and thus increase its foraging efficiency.[2]


References

  1. Raine, N.E.; Ings, T. C.; Dornhaus, A.; Saleh, N.; Chittka, L. (2006). "Adaptation, genetic drift, pleiotropy, and history in the evolution of bee foraging behavior". Advances in the Study of Behavior. 36: 305–354. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(06)36007-X. ISBN 9780120045365.
  2. Chittka, L.; Thomson, J.D.; Waser, N.M. (1999). "Flower constancy, insect psychology, and plant evolution". Naturwissenschaften. 86 (8): 361–177. doi:10.1007/s001140050636. S2CID 27377784.
  3. Waser, N.M. (1986). "Flower constancy: definition, cause and measurement". The American Naturalist. 127 (5): 596–603. doi:10.1086/284507.
  4. Hill, P.S.M.; Wells, P.H.; Wells, H. (1997). "Spontaneous flower constancy and learning in honey bees as a function of colour". Animal Behaviour. 54 (3): 615–627. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0467. PMID 9299046. S2CID 24674731.
  5. Stout, J.C.; Allen, J.A.; Goulson, D. (1998). "The influence of relative plant density and floral morphological complexity on the behaviour of bumblebees". Oecologia. 117 (4): 543–550. doi:10.1007/s004420050691. PMID 28307680. S2CID 5829708.
  6. Chittka, L.; Gumbert, A.; Kunze, J. (1997). "Foraging dynamics of bumble bees: correlates of movement within and between plant species". Behavioral Ecology. 8 (3): 239–249. doi:10.1093/beheco/8.3.239.
  7. Goulson, D.; Ollerton, J.; Sluman, C. (1997). "Foraging strategies in the small skipper butterfly, Thymelicus flavus: when to switch?". Animal Behaviour. 53 (5): 1009–1016. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0390. S2CID 620334.
  8. Harder, L. D., N.M. Williams, C.Y. Jordan, and W.A. Nelson. "The effects of Floral design and display on pollinator economics and pollen dispersal". 297-317. Editors, L. Chittka and J.D. Thomson. Cognitive Ecology of Pollination: Animal Behavior and Floral Evolution. 2001. Cambridge University Press
  9. Van Der Kooi, C. J.; Pen, I.; Staal, M.; Stavenga, D. G.; Elzenga, J. T. M. (2015). "Competition for pollinators and intra-communal spectral dissimilarity of flowers". Plant Biology. 18: 56–62. doi:10.1111/plb.12328. PMID 25754608.

Share this article:

This article uses material from the Wikipedia article Flower_constancy, and is written by contributors. Text is available under a CC BY-SA 4.0 International License; additional terms may apply. Images, videos and audio are available under their respective licenses.